
• The study reveals that decision-making and movement happen simultaneously, constantly adjusting to new sensory information rather than following a sequential process.
• Both humans and rats adjust their movements in real-time, demonstrating the brain’s flexibility in integrating past and present sensory input.
• These findings could improve adaptive AI algorithms and contribute to therapies for motor disorders by enhancing our understanding of movement and decision-making dynamics.
Imagine you’re at the grocery store, reaching for an apple. In that split second, you’re not just moving—you’re deciding. But what happens if new information arrives mid-reach? Do you commit to the motion you’ve started, or does your brain adjust on the fly?
A new study published in Nature Communications by researchers Manuel Molano-Mazón, Alexandre Garcia-Duran, Alexandre Hyafil, Jordi Pastor-Ciurana, Lluís Hernández-Navarro, Lejla Bektic, Debora Lombardo, and Jaime de la Rocha, explores this very question. The authors, affiliated with the Centre de Recerca Matemàtica (CRM), IDIBAPS, and the Universitat Politècnica de Catalunya (UPC), found that decision-making and movement don’t operate in separate steps. Instead, they unfold together, constantly adjusting in response to new sensory information.
For decades, scientists believed the brain followed a two-step process: first, deciding what to do, then figuring out how to do it. This study challenges that assumption. By observing humans and rats, the researchers discovered that decision-making and movement evolve in parallel, seamlessly integrating past expectations with real-time updates. “The brain is often treated as a machine that processes information sequentially: first, it decides, then it acts. But the reality is more complex. These processes are in constant communication and can modify each other in real-time,” explains Alexandre Garcia-Duran, a PhD student at CRM and one of the study’s authors.
In the experiment, human and animal participants had to choose between two options based on auditory cues. Sometimes, the environment confirmed their expectations; other times, it threw them a curveball. Initially, both rats and humans moved based on prior assumptions, predicting the likely outcome before fully processing the sensory evidence. But when the actual sound contradicted their expectation, they adapted—sometimes even reversing course mid-motion in what scientists call a change of mind.
For example, rats would begin moving toward a specific port, expecting the louder sound to come from that direction. Yet within just 100 milliseconds, if the evidence pointed elsewhere, they would either slow down, speed up or abruptly switch direction. Remarkably, human participants showed the same qualitative behaviour when performing a similar task on a touchscreen.
Humans, like rats, balance prior expectations with real-time sensory input. Their accuracy improved as sound cues became clearer, even when reacting in as little as 50 milliseconds. When expectation and evidence aligned, movements were faster; when they clashed, movements slowed down. In about 7% of trials, participants changed their decision mid-movement—most often when prior assumptions were strongly contradicted by new information.
“A change of mind occurs when two sources of information contradict each other. Your expectations tell you one thing, but new evidence tells you another. If the new signal is strong enough and the cost of adapting is low, you switch,” says Garcia-Duran. He adds that it’s not just about how much information the brain receives but also the effort required to change course: “If you’re about to complete a movement and a contradictory signal arrives, you might just finish what you started instead of correcting mid-action.”
The Brain’s Parallel Processing Power
To understand this dynamic interplay between movement and decision-making, researchers built a computational model. Unlike traditional models that only predict final choices, this one captures how decisions evolve in real-time and shape movement.

The model accumulates sensory evidence through a drift-diffusion process, integrating prior expectations and real-time stimuli to determine movement direction. An urgency signal triggers an initial movement, which can later be updated based on further sensory input, either accelerating or reversing the trajectory if the new evidence contradicts prior assumptions. To estimate model parameters, a neural network is trained on 10 million simulations, approximating choice, reaction time, and movement adjustments for likelihood-based inference.
“ Richard Feynman once said: ‘What I cannot create, I do not understand.’ That’s essentially what we do with models—break down the fundamental mechanisms that drive behaviour,” Garcia-Duran explains. The model outlines two pivotal stages: an initial read-out, where movement is triggered by prior expectations and minimal sensory input, and a second read-out, where fresh sensory data fine-tunes the action. If the new evidence sharply contradicts the initial decision, the brain can even reverse the movement mid-action—an adaptive skill essential for survival in unpredictable environments.
To test the model’s accuracy, the team trained artificial neural networks on millions of simulated decisions, predicting choices, reaction times, and movement speeds. The results aligned closely with real-world behaviour: movements were faster when expectations matched sensory input and slower when they conflicted. This supports the idea that our actions aren’t pre-programmed sequences, but dynamic processes shaped by an ongoing accumulation of evidence.
However, Garcia-Duran emphasises that no model is perfect: “Models are never an exact replica of reality. Human behaviour is incredibly stochastic—there are so many overlapping processes that some level of variability is inevitable. The challenge is to isolate the fundamental mechanisms while keeping the model simple enough to be useful.”
Understanding how the brain continuously refines movement has implications far beyond the lab. These insights could improve robotic motion algorithms, making artificial intelligence more adaptive in real-world environments. They could also inform therapies for motor disorders like Parkinson’s disease, where both movement control and decision-making are impaired.
“The most immediate potential application is in clinical research. If we understand how this process should work under normal conditions, we can compare it with patients who have neurological disorders and identify what’s malfunctioning,” Garcia-Duran points out.
Looking ahead, the researchers want to explore whether the brain continuously integrates new information or does so in discrete steps. “One big question is whether we are constantly processing new evidence or updating in discrete bursts. What’s more efficient; absorbing all incoming information at all times or selectively updating when necessary? That’s something we still don’t know,” Garcia-Duran says.
So, the next time you hesitate mid-action or suddenly change course, don’t mistake it for indecision. It’s just your brain doing what it does best; keeping you on the right path, even when the world throws you new information at the last second.
Article reference:
Molano-Mazón, M., Garcia-Duran, A., Pastor-Ciurana, J. et al. Rapid, systematic updating of movement by accumulated decision evidence. Nat Commun 15, 10583 (2024). https://doi.org/10.1038/s41467-024-53586-7
|
CRM CommPau Varela
|
Núria Fagella, nova degana de la Facultat de Matemàtiques i Informàtica de la UB
La professora i investigadora Núria Fagella ha estat escollida nova degana electa de la Facultat de Matemàtiques i Informàtica de la Universitat de Barcelona. Especialista en dinàmica complexa i adscrita al Centre de Recerca Matemàtica, el seu...
El CRM apropa el cervell al públic a la Festa de la Ciència amb l’activitat “Neural Code”
Aquest cap de setmana, el Centre de Recerca Matemàtica ha participat a la 18a Festa de la Ciència de Barcelona amb l’activitat Neural Code, un taller interactiu per explorar com el cervell transforma els senyals neuronals en moviment. L’activitat, organitzada en...
The CRM hosts the Advanced Course on Modern Aspects of Fourier Analysis
From May 26 to May 30, 2025, the Centre de Recerca Matemàtica (CRM) in Bellaterra welcomed more than 80 participants from institutions around the world for the Advanced Course on Modern Aspects of Fourier Analysis. The activity, held at the CRM Auditorium, was one of...
Could the Sun–Jupiter–Saturn System Truly Exist? A Computer-Assisted Approach Sheds New Light on Celestial Stability
Mathematicians Jordi-Lluís Figueras and Alex Haro have taken a step toward validating a long-standing astronomical hypothesis: that the Sun–Jupiter–Saturn system could sustain stable, quasiperiodic motion indefinitely, even in the face of chaotic...
CRM publishes its 2024 Annual Report
The Centre de Recerca Matemàtica (CRM) has released its 2024 Annual Report, a comprehensive overview of the centre’s scientific output, institutional developments, training initiatives, and outreach activities over the past year. This edition carries particular...
BARCCSYN 2025: 12th Edition of Barcelona’s Leading Computational and Systems Neuroscience Gathering
On May 22 and 23, the Institut d’Estudis Catalans (IEC) opened its doors to the 12th edition of the Barcelona Computational, Cognitive and Systems Neuroscience (BARCCSYN) conference. Now a well-established event in the city’s scientific calendar, BARCCSYN continues to...
Celebrating the International Day of Women in Mathematics with Six Voices from the CRM
To celebrate the International Day of Women in Mathematics, the Centre de Recerca Matemàtica (CRM) shares a video featuring six inspiring researchers—Marta Casanellas, Marta Mazzocco, Gissell Estrada, Núria Fagella, Joana Cirici, and Gloria Cecchini. Through their...
Mercuri i Venus: Les Danses Orbitals Sense Inclinació
L'article de Clarke i Guàrdia explora la raó per la qual els planetes interiors, com Mercuri i Venus, mantenen òrbites gairebé planes en comparació amb altres cossos celestes. Els investigadors han demostrat que l'estabilitat marginal dels sistemes planetaris només és...
Dos investigadors del CRM, reconeguts amb premis de la Societat Catalana de Matemàtiques i l’IEC
Els investigadors del Centre de Recerca Matemàtica Robert Cardona i Joaquim Duran han estat reconeguts amb dos guardons dels Premis Matemàtics IEC 2025. Cardona ha rebut el Premi IEC de Matemàtiques per la seva tesi sobre la geometria dels fluids...
New Mathematical Model Helps Improve Filtration of Competing Contaminants
Researchers from the CRM, Universitat Politècnica de Catalunya, and Universitat de Girona have developed a mathematical model to better understand how different air pollutants compete for space in filtration systems. Using a travelling wave...
Peregrina Quintela reconeguda amb el Premi Julio Peláez 2025
La Dra. Peregrina Quintela Estévez, membre del Comitè Científic Assessor del Centre de Recerca Matemàtica (CRM) des del 2017, ha estat distingida amb el Premi Julio Peláez 2025 a Científiques amb Lideratge Cívic. El guardó reconeix la seva trajectòria en matemàtiques...
Chenchang Zhu, Inaugural CRM–María de Maeztu Chair of Excellence
The Centre de Recerca Matemàtica (CRM) has named Professor Chenchang Zhu (University of Göttingen) as the first María de Maeztu Chair of Excellence, a position that recognises leading women mathematicians. Her appointment began on April 9, 2025, with a lecture on...